The Influence of Emotion on Inter-Temporal Choice: Based on Evaluation Tendency Framework Theory
Abstract
Emotion is an important influencing factor in inter-temporal choice. The evaluation tendency framework theory explains the impact of different specific emotions on the inter-temporal choice overriding the valence. It is also considered as the important basic theory of a specific emotional paradigm. The thesis summarizes the formulation of this theory, the connotation, the assumption of neural mechanisms, and the application of the theory in inter-temporal choice research. It is pointed out that the theory can extend its research in terms of the specificity of emotion, the influence of time perception and so on by using neurocognitive science, and further explore the influence of different specific emotions on different inter-temporal choice and their internal psychological mechanisms.
Keywords
Full Text:
PDFReferences
Augustine, A. A., & Larsen, R. J. (2011). Affect regulation and temporal discounting: Interactions between primed, state, and trait affect. Emotion, 11(2), 403-12.
Baucells, M., & Heukamp. F. (2012). Probability and time trade-off. Management Science, 58, 831-842.
Benzion, U., Rapoport, A., & Yagil, J. (1989). Discount rates inferred from decisions: An experimental study. Management Science, 35, 270-284.
Betancourt, H., & Blair, I. (1992). A cognition (attribution)-emotion model of violence in conflict situations. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 18, 343-350.
Biel, A., Johansson-Stenman, O., & Nilsson, A. (2011). The willingness to pay-willingness to accept gap revisited: The role of emotions and moral satisfaction. Journal of Economic Psychology, 32, 908-917.
Bodenhausen, G. V., Kramer, G. P., & Süsser, K. (1994). Happiness and stereotypic thinking in social judgment. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 66(4), 621-632.
Carte, D. M. (2011). Recognizing the role of positive emotions in fostering environmentally responsible behaviors. Ecopsychology, 3, 65-69.
Chen, H. X., & He, G. B. (2011). The impact of understanding level on intertemporal choice and risk selection. Acta Psychological Science, 43(4), 442-452.
Chen, H. X., He, G. B. (2014). The influence of psychological distance on intertemporal choice and risk selection. Acta Psychological Science, 46(5), 677-690.
Cryder, C. E., Lerner, J. S., Gross, J. J., & Re, D. (2008). Misery is not miserly: Sad and self-focused individuals spend more. Psychological Science, 19(6), 525-30.
Damen, F. (2009). Searing sentiment or cold calculation? The effects of leader emotional displays on team performance depend on follower epistemic motivation. Academy of Management Journal, 52(3), 562-580.
Davidson, R. J., Pizzagalli, D., Nitschke, J. B., &Putnam, K. (2002). Depression: Perspectives from affective neuroscience. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 545-574.
Ding Daoqun, & Zhang Xiangyi. (2013). Emotion as a “catalyst” of moral judgment: evaluation tendency and specific effect in moral judgment. Psychology Exploring new, 33(6), 489-493.
Elster, J. (1998). Emotions and economic theory. Journal of Economic Literature, 36, 47-74.
Epper, T., Fehr-Duda, H., & Bruhin, A. (2011). Viewing the future through a warped lens: why uncertainty generates hyperbolic discounting. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 43(3), 169-203.
Halevy, Y. (2008). Strotz meets Allais: Diminishing impatience and the certainty effect. American Economic Review, 98, 1145-1162.
Harle, K. M., & Sanfey, A. G. (2007). Incidental sadness biases social economic decisions in the ultimatum game. Emotion, 7, 876-881.
Hastie, P. (2001). Problems for judgment and decision making. Annual Review of Psychology, 52(1), 653-683.
Higgins, E. T. (1997). Beyond pleasure and pain. American Psychologist, 52, 1280-1300.
Ifcher, J., & Zarghamee, H. (2011). Happiness and time preference: The effect of positive affect in a random-assignment experiment. American Economic Review, 101(7), 3109-3129.
Jin, S. P., & Isen, A. M. (2011). Positive affect, intertemporal choice, and levels of thinking: Increasing consumers’ willingness to wait. Journal of Marketing Research, 48(3), 532-543.
Kable, J. W., & Glimcher, P. W. (2007). The neural correlates of subjective value during intertemporal choice. Nature Neuroscience, 10(12), 1625-1633.
Kable, J. W., & Glimcher, P. W. (2010). An “as soon as possible” effect in human intertemporal decision making: Behavioral evidence and neural mechanisms. Journal of neurophysiology, 3, 2513-2531.
Keller, C., Bostrom, A., Kuttschreuter, M., Savadori, L., Spence, A., & White, M. (2012). Bringing appraisal theory to environmental risk perception: a review of conceptual approaches of the past 40 years and suggestions for future research. Journal of Risk Research, 7(6), 506-511.
Kim, S., Hwang, J., & Lee, D. (2008). Prefrontal coding of temporally discounted values during intertemporal choice. Neuron, 59, 161-172.
Kirby, K. N., & Herrnstein, R. J. (1995). Preference reversals due to myopic discounting of delayed reward. Psychological Science, 6(2), 83-89.
Laibson, D. I. (1997). Golden eggs and hyperbolic discounting. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 112, 443-477.
Lazarus, R. S. (1991a). Emotion and adaptation. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Lazarus, R. S. (1991b). Progress on a cognitive-motivational-relational theory of emotion. American Psychologist, 46, 819-834.
Lempert, K. M., Glimcher, P. W., & Phelps, E. A. (2015). Emotional arousal and discount rate in intertemporal choice are reference dependent. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 144(2), 366-373.
Lerner, J. S., & Keltner, D. (2000). Beyond valence: Toward a model of emotion-specific influences on judgment and choice. Cognition & Emotion, 14(4), 473-493.
Lerner, J. S., & Tiedens, L. Z. (2006). Portrait of an angry decision maker: How appraisal tendencies shape anger’s influence on cognition. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 19(2), 115-137.
Lerner, J. S., Gonzalez, R. M., Small, D. A., & Fischhoff, B. (2003). Effects of fear and anger on perceived risks of terrorism: A national field experiment. Psychological Science, 14(2), 144-150(7).
Lerner, J. S., Li, Y.,& Weber, E. (2012). The financial cost of sadness. General Information, 24(1), 72-79.
Lerner, J. S., Small, D. A., & Loewenstein, G. (2004). Heart strings and Purse strings: Carryover effects of emotions on economic transactions. Psychological Science, 15(5), 337-341.
Levenson, R. (1994). Human emotion: A functional view. In P. Ekman & R. J. Davidson (Eds.), The nature of emotion (pp. 123-126). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Li, A. M., Sun, H. L., Xiong, G. X., Wang, X. T., & Li, B. (2016). The influence of “time poverty” on intertemporal decision-making and forward-looking behavior and its cognitive mechanism. Advances in Psychological Science, 24(6), 874-884.
Li, J., Huang, R. H., & Zeng, X. Q. (2015). The impact of uncertainty tolerance on intertemporal choice and its context dependence. Psychological Science (3), 680-685.
Loewenstein, G. F., Weber, E. U., Hsee, C. K., & Welch, N. (2001). Risk as feelings. Psychological Bulletin, 127(2).267-86.
Luo, S., Ainslie, G., & Monterosso, J. (2014). The behavioral and neural effect of emotional primes on intertemporal decisions. Social Cognitive & Affective Neuroscience, 9(3), 283-291.
Luo, S., Ainslie, G., Pollini, D., Giragosian, L., & Monterosso, J. R. (2012). Moderators of the association between brain activation and farsighted choice. Neuroimage, 59(2), 1469-1477.
Ma, W. J., Suo, T., Li, Y. D., Luo, W., Feng, T. Y., & Li, H., et al. (2012). Separation of the framework effect of gains and losses——from the study of income and loss type intertemporal choice. Acta Psychological Science, 44(8), 1038- 1046.
Mazur, J. E. (1984). Tests of an equivalence rule for fixed and variable reinforcer delays. Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior, 10, 426-436.
Mcclure, S. M., Laibson, D. I., Loewenstein, G., & Cohen, J. D. (2004). Separate neural systems value immediate and delayed monetary rewards. Science, 306(5695), 503-507.
Miller, E. K., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24, 167-202.
Oatley, K., & Johnson, P. N. (1996). The communicative theory of emotions: Empirical tests, mental models, and implications for social interaction.
Patak, M., & Reynolds, B. (2007). Question-based assessments of delay discounting: Do respondents spontaneously incorporate uncertainty into their valuations for delayed rewards?. Addictive Behaviors, 32(2), 351-357.
Pulcu, E., Trotter, P. D., Thomas, E. J., Mcfarquhar, M., Juhasz, G., & Sahakian, B. J. (2014). Temporal discounting in major depressive disorder. Psychological Medicine, 44(9), 1825-1834.
Pyone, J. S., & Isen, A. M. (2011). Positive affect, intertemporal choice, and levels of thinking: Increasing consumers’ willingness to wait. Journal of Marketing research, 48, 532-543.
Ratner, R. K, & Herbst, K. C. (2005). When good decisions have bad outcomes: The impact of affect on switching behavior. Organ Behave Hum Decis Process, 96, 22-27.
Read, D., & Roelofsma, P. (2003). Subadditive versus hyperbolic discounting: A comparison of choice and matching. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 91, 140-153.
Rounds, J. S., Beck, J. G., & Grant, D. M. M. (2007). Is the delay discounting paradigm useful in understanding social anxiety?. Behaviour Research & Therapy, 45(4), 729-735.
Samuelson, P. A. (1937). A note on measurement of utility. The Review of Economic Studies, 4, 155-161.
Scherer, K. R. (1988). Facets of emotion: Recent research. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Scholten, M., & Read, D. (2010). The psychology of intertemporal tradeoffs. Psychological Review, 117(3), 925-944.
She, S. X., Zheng, X. W., Zhou, W., & Yang, W. (2016). Does fear reduce the patience of intertemporal choices?——Evidence from behavioral experiments. Psychology Exploring new, 36(1), 25-30.
She, S., Ma, C., &Wu, D. (2010). General probability-time trade-off and intertemporal risk-value model. Risk Analysis, 30, 421-431.
Shiv, B. (2007). Emotions, decisions, and the brain. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 17(3), 174-178.
Slovic, P., Finucane, M., Peters, E., & Macgregor, D. G. (2002). Rational actors or rational fools: Implications of the affect heuristic for behavioral economics. The Journal of Socio-Economics, 31(4), 329-342.
Small, D. A., & Lerner, J. S. (2008). Emotional policy: Personal sadness and anger shape judgments about a welfare case. Political Psychology, 29(2), 149-168.
Smith, C. A., & Ellsworth, P. C. (1985). Patterns of cognitive appraisal in emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 813-838.
Sohn, J. H., Kim, H. E., Sohn, S., Seok, J. W., Choi, D., & Watanuki, S. (2015). Effect of emotional arousal on inter-temporal decision-making: An fmri study. Journal of Physiological Anthropology, 34(1), 1-8.
Suo, T., Zhang, F., Zhao, G. X., & Li, H. (2014). The effect of time perception differences on intertemporal selection propensity. Acta Psychological Science, 46(2), 165-173.
Takahashi, T., Oono, H., Inoue, T., Boku, S., Kako, Y., Kitaichi, Y., Kusumi, I., Masui, T., Nakagawa, S., Suzuki, K., Tanaka, T., Koyama, T., Radford, M. H. (2008). Depressive patients are more impulsive and inconsistent in intertemporal choice behavior for monetary gain and loss than healthy subjects- an analysis based on tsallis’ statistics. Neuro Endocrinology Letters, 29(3), 351-8.
Tao Anqi, Liu Jinping, Feng Tingyong. (2015). Time Insight for Predicting Intertemporal Choice Preference. Psychological Science, 38(2), 278-283.
Thaler, R. (1981). Some empirical evidence on dynamic inconsistency. Economics Letters, 8, 201-207.
Van Kleef, G. A., Homan, A. C., Beersma, B., Van Knippenberg, D., Van Knippenberg, B., & Soman, D., Ainslie, G., Frederick, S., Li, X., Lynch, J., & Moreau, P. (2005). The psychology of intertemporal discounting: Why are distant events valued differently from proximal ones? Marketing Letters, 16(3), 347-360.
Wang, P., & Liu, Y. F. (2009). The Influence of Emotion on Intertemporal Choice. Psychological Science (6), 1318-1320.
Xu, F. M., Zhang, H., Wu, X. L., Li, B., & Luo, H. B. (2014). New orientation of emotional and decision-making relationships beyond potency. Psychological Research, 7(2), 9-16.
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.3968/10952
Refbacks
- There are currently no refbacks.
Copyright (c) 2019 Canadian Social Science
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.
Reminder
- How to do online submission to another Journal?
- If you have already registered in Journal A, then how can you submit another article to Journal B? It takes two steps to make it happen:
1. Register yourself in Journal B as an Author
- Find the journal you want to submit to in CATEGORIES, click on “VIEW JOURNAL”, “Online Submissions”, “GO TO LOGIN” and “Edit My Profile”. Check “Author” on the “Edit Profile” page, then “Save”.
2. Submission
Online Submission: http://cscanada.org/index.php/css/submission/wizard
- Go to “User Home”, and click on “Author” under the name of Journal B. You may start a New Submission by clicking on “CLICK HERE”.
- We only use four mailboxes as follows to deal with issues about paper acceptance, payment and submission of electronic versions of our journals to databases: [email protected]; [email protected]; [email protected]; [email protected]
Articles published in Canadian Social Science are licensed under Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 (CC-BY).
Canadian Social Science Editorial Office
Address: 1020 Bouvier Street, Suite 400, Quebec City, Quebec, G2K 0K9, Canada.
Telephone: 1-514-558 6138
Website: Http://www.cscanada.net; Http://www.cscanada.org
E-mail:[email protected]; [email protected]
Copyright © Canadian Academy of Oriental and Occidental Culture