Beyond Age and Adjustment: A Cross-National Qualitative Study of Older Adults’ Perceptions
Sofia von Humboldt[a],*; Isabel Leal[a]; Filipa Pimenta[a]; Georgeta Niculescu[b]
[a] Research Unit in Psychology and Health, R&D, ISPA – Instituto Universitário, Lisbon, Portugal.
[b] Georgeta Niculescu: Romanian Association of Person-Centered Psychotherapy, Bucharest, Romania.
*Corresponding author.
Address: Research Unit in Psychology and Health, R&D, ISPA – Instituto Universitário. Rua Jardim do Tabaco, 34, 1149-041 Lisboa, Portugal.
Supported by the Portuguese Foundation for Science and Technology (FCT), for the grant [grant number SFRH/BD/44544/2008] which supported this research. Sponsor’s Role: The fund approved the design and aims of the study but did not play any role in the collecting of data, interpretation of results, or preparation of this article.
Received 20 August 2012; accepted 5 October 2012
Abstract
Aims: To explore the older adults’ perceptions of age and aging, focusing on adjustment to aging (AtA) and subjective age (SA). Methods: This cross-national and qualitative study comprised demographics and semi-structured interviews. Complete information on 151 older adults aged between 75-101 years (M = 84.6; SD = 6.905) from Portugal, Romania Angola was available. Data was subjected to content analysis. Results: The most predominant response of the interviewed participants for indicators of AtA was ‘existential meaning’ (26.3%), whilst ‘balanced’ (36.5%) was identified as the most prevalent SA response. In total, five categories were identified to be indicative of AtA: ‘sense of purpose and ambitions’, ‘health and wellness’, ‘social support’, ‘stability and accessibility’ and ‘existential meaning’ whereas, four categories were identified for SA: ‘balanced, ‘old’, ‘youthful’ and ‘dissatisfied’. Conclusions: This study highlights the need for a better understanding of what defines AtA and SA among the elderly. Furthermore, health care providers’ awareness of older adults’ conceptualizations will allow them to communicate more effectively and to reinforce aging well among older populations.
Key words: Adjustment to aging; subjective age; aging well; older adults; content analysis
Sofia von Humboldt, Isabel Leal, Filipa Pimenta & Georgeta Niculescu (2012). Beyond Age and Adjustment: A Cross-National Qualitative Study of Older Adults’ Perceptions. Canadian Social Science, 8(5), 145-153. Available from: http://www.cscanada.net/index.php/css/article/view/j.css.1923669720120805.5541
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.3968/j.css.1923669720120805.5541.
Introduction
The dramatic increases in life expectancy over the past century have created a number of challenges for society as its members age. Countries from Europe and North America have the oldest populations in the world, thus, placing research into the effects and implications of an aging population in a unique position. Conversely, little attention has been paid to African older adults, in part because of the fact that older people still make up a relatively small fraction of the total African population. However, demographic projections bolster a case for greater attention to be given to the situation of older Africans (United Nations Population Division, 1999). Indeed, the older population (60 years and older) of sub-Saharan Africa is projected to increase from 30.4 million to 56.5 million in the next 25 years, hence, reliable research is needed to inform policy planners and health and social welfare service providers (United States Bureau of the Census, 2000). Additionally, on the basis of increasing life expectancy and the decline in fertility, it is predicted that half century from now, Europe will have in total, 300 millions inhabitants over 60 (Fernández-Ballesteros, 2007; United Nations, Department of Economic and Social Affairs, Population Division, 2011). In particular, the proportion of individuals aged 80 or over is projected to rise from one percent to four percent of the global population between today and 2050 (Gavrilov & Heuveline, 2003; United Nations, 2009).
As adults experience their older years, many aspects of life (social, financial, physical, and employment) change (Corner, Brittain, & Bond, 2007). Moreover, previous literature emphasizes the fact that cultural, national and ethnical differences may influence the process of aging (Barak, 2009; Torres, 2003) and that aging is an ongoing process which requires continuous adjustment (Birren & Schaie, 1996; Brozek, 1966).
Furthermore, the selective optimization with compensation model suggests that successful aging corresponds to a general process of adjustment, in which the three elements – selection, optimization and compensation – constitute the basic component processes for changes regarding aging and adaptive capacity (Baltes & Smith, 2003; Freund & Baltes, 1998). Additionally, Brandtstädter and Renner (1990) found that older cohorts tended to report more accommodative coping strategies than younger ones. In other words, they adjust themselves rather than the environment to cope with problems (Brandtstädter & Renner, 1990; Brandtstädter, Wentura, & Greve, 1993; Filion, Wister, & Coblentz, 1993).
Adjustment to aging (AtA) it is a multidimensional and multi-cultural concept (Barak, 2009; Bauer & McAdams, 2004; Keyes, Shmotkin, & Ryff, 2002; Slangen-Dekort, Midden, Aarts, & Wagenberg, 2001; Staudinger & Kunzmann, 2005), which is achieved when a balance between the cognitive and motivational systems of the person has been attained (Thomae, 1992). Specifically, AtA is reached by balancing one’s own experience, self-standards, personal aims, core motivations and values with external influences in possiblepanoply of issues which shed light on the adaptation to growing old (Headey & Wearing, 1989; Heidrich & Ryff, 1993; Keyes, Shmotkin, 1998; Shmotkin, & Ryff, 2002). Moreover, in a more recent study, Jopp and Rott (2006) pointed out that basic resources (e.g., cognition, health, extraversion), self-referent beliefs (e.g., self-efficacy) and attitudes toward life (e.g., optimistic outlook) were pertinent for AtA in old age. Additionally, Sneed and Whitbourne (2003) conceptualized it, according to identity process theory. Indeed, it is a proximate, yet distinct, from other well-being concepts previously defined and validated in the gerontological literature, such as psychological well-being and successful aging (Fernández-Ballesteros, 2010; Kesebir & Diener, 2010; Keyes, Shmotkin, & Ryff, 2002; Low & Molzahn, 2007; Pavot & Diener, 2004, von Humboldt, Leal, & Pimenta, 2012). As regards to psychological well-being, it entails the perception of engagement with existential challenges of life and human development and comprises six psychological components: self-acceptance, environmental mastery, autonomy, purpose in life, personal growth and self-acceptance (Keyes, Shmotkin, & Ryff, 2002; Ryff & Keyes, 1995), whereas successful aging is conceptualized as a multifaceted construct which includes constituents of life satisfaction, morale, adjustment, quality of life, and social functioning in psychological approaches (Baltes & Baltes, 1993; Kuh, 2007) and physical and cognitive functioning in biomedical approaches (Ford et al., 2000; Park, Gutchess, Meade, & Stine-Morrow, 2007). Furthermore, Knight and Ricciardelli’s study (2003), highlights other possible determinants of one’ perception of what aging successfully means, such as lifelong personal experiences, religiosity, one’s attitude to aging and to life in general, and personality (especially) in respect of rigidity versus openness to change).
Overall, Western cultures have been said to celebrate youth and to devalue old age (Palmore, 2001). This has also been found to be reflected in expressions of subjective age (SA) while from middle age onwards adults have been shown to resist their chronological age and report a discrepancy between their actual and subjective ages (Westerhof & Barrett, 2005). The perception of SA, or how young or old individuals experience themselves to be (Barak, 2009, Montepare, 2009) can become a significant challenge to their notions of identity and congruence (Baltes & Smith, 2003; Kleinspehn-Ammerlahn, Kotter-Grühn, & Smith, 2008). In addition, SA was found to contribute to subjective health, memory self-efficacy, and life satisfaction, regardless of chronological age (Stephan, Caudroit, & Chalabaev, 2011).
Despite the fact that AtA and SA are different concepts in gerontological literature, they are related to the multi-dimensional and multi-cultural context of adjustment and age (Barak, 2009; Torres, 2003). We assert that the deepening of these relevant concepts can become a significant means for health care interventions, thus, improving well-being and longevity (Staudinger & Kunzmann, 2005), as the sole use of health gain measures may neglect potential benefits (Al-Janabi, Flynn, & Coast, 2012).
Considering that the experience of age and aging is subjective and may have different meanings for different people, qualitative research allows gaining access to the personal perceptions and experiences of older adults, in alternative to formulating a logical or scientific explanation based on previous theories. Hence, with this paper we aim at contributing with a baseline for further research, focusing on older adults’ self-reports concerning AtA and SA. Specifically, this is a qualitative study, designed to: (a) analyze older adults’ perceptions of AtA and SA, through major categories that had impact on their conceptualization of AtA and SA and (c) assess differences among the three nationalities, concerning AtA and SA.
Methods
Participants
One hundred and fifty one eligible non-institutionalized, nationally-diverse individuals, aged 75 and over (M=84.6; SD=6.905; range 75-101) were interviewed and questioned about their perceived AtA and SA. Participants were recruited through senior universities’ message boards, local and art community centres member lists, in Lisbon, Bucharest and in the Algarve regions.
The sampling of participants was based on the availability of respondents. Participant eligibility included: (1) 75 years of age or older and (2) participants’ score in the normal range on the Mini Mental Status Exam (>26) (Folstein, Folstein, & McHugh, 1975). None of the participants had any history of psychiatric or neurological illness, or history of drug or alcohol abuse, which might compromise cognitive function. Table 1 shows the characteristics of the study’s participants.
Table 1
Distribution of the Study’s Participants According to Sociodemographic and Health-Related Characteristics
Portuguese |
Romanian |
Angolan |
Total |
|||||
N |
% |
N |
% |
N |
% |
N |
% |
|
N |
52 |
34.4 |
51 |
33.8 |
48 |
31.8 |
151 |
100.0 |
Age(M;SD) |
86.0(7.387) |
84.6(6.806) |
83.0(6.243) |
84.6(6.905) |
||||
Gender |
||||||||
Male |
24 |
46.2 |
20 |
39.2 |
19 |
39.6 |
63 |
41.7 |
Female |
28 |
53.8 |
31 |
60.8 |
29 |
60.4 |
88 |
58.3 |
Education |
||||||||
Primaryschool |
6 |
11.5 |
3 |
5.9 |
9 |
18.8 |
18 |
11.9 |
Middle school |
16 |
30.8 |
10 |
19.6 |
16 |
33.3 |
42 |
27.8 |
High school |
23 |
44.2 |
20 |
39.2 |
11 |
22.9 |
54 |
35.8 |
University degree or higher |
7 |
13.5 |
18 |
35.3 |
12 |
25.0 |
37 |
24.5 |
Marital Status |
||||||||
Marriedorinarelationship |
38 |
73.1 |
34 |
66.7 |
30 |
62.5 |
102 |
67.5 |
Single |
7 |
13.5 |
6 |
11.8 |
2 |
4.2 |
15 |
9.9 |
Widowed |
7 |
13.4 |
11 |
21.5 |
16 |
33.3 |
34 |
22.6 |
Professional Status |
||||||||
Active |
22 |
42.3 |
22 |
43.1 |
21 |
43.8 |
65 |
43.0 |
Inactive |
30 |
57.7 |
29 |
56.9 |
27 |
56.2 |
86 |
57.0 |
Family Annual Income |
||||||||
≤20,000€ |
27 |
51.9 |
37 |
72.5 |
39 |
81.3 |
103 |
68.2 |
≥20,001€ |
25 |
48.1 |
14 |
27.5 |
9 |
18.7 |
48 |
31.8 |
Perceived Health |
||||||||
Good |
29 |
55.8 |
32 |
62.7 |
32 |
66.7 |
93 |
61.6 |
Poor |
23 |
44.2 |
19 |
37.3 |
16 |
33.3 |
58 |
38.4 |
Note: Total sample, n = 151; SD = standard deviation.
Measures and Procedure
Face-to-face semi-structured interviews lasting between 30 and 45 minutes were performed with each participant. The initial 10–15 minutes of each interview were used to apply the general questionnaire and to assess demographic and clinical data.
Our interview schedule was general and open-ended. It was composed by two open-ended questions: “How do you feel about your age?” and “I would like to understand what in your point of view, contributes to your adjustment to aging in this phase of your life.” All interviews were digitally-recorded verbatim and then transcribed to typed format for analysis.
All the participants’ responses were subjected to qualitative content analysis, using the following procedure: (a) definition of major emergent categories, mutually exclusive, for each one of the three pre-existing categories (indicators of AtA and SA); (b) creation of a list of coding cues; (c) analysis of verbatim quotes of participants’ narratives that better link to emerging categories; (e) identification of sub-categories, while preserving the principle of homogeneity of the category; (f) derivation of emergent categories, through constant comparison within and across interviews allowing for the clustering of related sub-categories until the point of theoretical saturation was reached (Bardin, 2007).
Our structure of sub-categories and categories was then subjected to an external review and critical feedback was obtained from reviewers with experience with older adults. An independent analysis of the interviews was performed by a jury of two psychologists (both faculty) and a final group co-resolution regarding the categories was made. Data were analyzed using SPSS for Windows (version 19.0; SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL).
The Portuguese Foundation for Science and Technology (FCT) and ISPA - Instituto Universitário, approved the study. Informed consent was received from all participants and the study protocol was approved by the Research Unit in Psychology and Health’s coordination.
Results
Content Analysis of the Emergent Categories
Adjustment to Aging
Findings indicated a total of five categories of indicators of AtA: (1) ‘fulfilment and ambitions’; (2) ‘health and wellness’; (3) ‘social support’; (4) ‘stability and accessibility’ and (5) ‘existential meaning’. ‘Existential meaning’ (26.3%) was the most mentioned indicator of AtA, as evidenced in Table 2.
Table 2
Emergent Categories Resulting from Content Analysis of the Pre-Category ‘indicators of AtA’
Portuguese |
Romanian |
Angolan |
Total |
|||||
N |
% |
N |
% |
N |
% |
N |
% |
|
Indicators of AtA |
||||||||
Sense of Purpose and Ambitions |
63 |
24.3 |
27 |
14.3 |
69 |
33.1 |
159 |
24.2 |
Health and Wellness |
43 |
16.6 |
34 |
18.0 |
38 |
18.5 |
115 |
17.6 |
Social Support |
34 |
13.1 |
35 |
18.5 |
40 |
19.2 |
109 |
16.6 |
Existential Meaning |
76 |
29.3 |
56 |
29.6 |
41 |
19.5 |
173 |
26.3 |
Stability and Accessibility |
43 |
16.7 |
37 |
19.6 |
20 |
9.8 |
100 |
15.3 |
Score of pre-category ‘indicators of AtA’ |
259 |
100.0 |
189 |
100.0 |
208 |
100.0 |
656 |
100.0 |
a) Sense of Purpose and Ambitions
Participants reported their deeds (e.g., volunteering, teaching) and ambitions, such as future projects as indicators of AtA. Participants also referred that being engaged with a creative work or occupation was relevant for their AtA, as it brought them a sense of purpose.
“I feel alive every time I go to the hospital. I know that I can contribute with something there.” (Participant 121)
“Teaching and communicating with my students makes me think of solutions. This keeps me young.” (Participant 19)
b) Health and Wellness
Participants verbalized being healthy and keeping their cognitive autonomy as pertinent for their AtA. Moreover, body appearance and outdoor sports were relevant for these participants.
“I feel well about my body but I know that I am already 78 so I have to be careful with everything, such as a simple cold.” (Participant 32)
“I walk two kilometers every day. I do it at my own rhythm and it’s a time that I dedicate to my thoughts.” (Participant 11)
c) Social Support
Family, neighbours, friends and professional peers were indicated as the main sources of support and main indicators of AtA.
“My neighbours help me a lot. They bring me the groceries every week.” (Participant 39)
“I enjoy talking to my colleagues. We all know each other for so long that in a way I shared more time with them than with my own family (Participant 143)
d) Existential Meaning
Older adults expressed their concerns towards death of dear ones and their own. Moreover, they reported their spiritual activities, such as yoga and reiki as relevant for their AtA. Some participants also verbalized their religious beliefs as pertinent for their AtA.
“Believing in myself and in others makes me feel well about my life and age.” (Participant 121)
“I enjoy the fact that I belong to our Christian religious community.” (Participant 149)
e) Stability and Accessibility
Participants considered financial stability and independence as relevant for their AtA. Moreover living in an accessible and comfortable environment was also reported as important for their AtA.
“I have everything in my neighbourhood. I know that I do not have to drive anywhere.” (Participant 67)
“My house is like my nest. I do not depend on anyone.” (Participant 115)
Subjective Age
Findings suggested five emergent categories of answers for SA: (a) ‘balanced, (b) ‘old’, (d) ‘youthful’ and (e) ‘dissatisfied’. ‘Balanced’ (36.5%) was the most mentioned SA, as seen in Table 3.
Table 3
Emergent Categories Resulting from Content Analysis of the Pre-Category ‘Subjective Age’
Portuguese |
Romanian |
Angolan |
Total |
|||||
N |
% |
N |
% |
N |
% |
N |
% |
|
Subjective Age |
||||||||
Balanced |
68 |
46.9 |
34 |
23.6 |
58 |
38.9 |
160 |
36.5 |
Old |
35 |
24.1 |
49 |
34.0 |
10 |
6.7 |
94 |
21.5 |
Youthful |
28 |
19.3 |
34 |
23.6 |
67 |
45.5 |
129 |
29.6 |
Dissatisfied |
14 |
9.7 |
27 |
18.8 |
13 |
8.9 |
54 |
12.4 |
Score of pre-category ‘subjective age’ |
145 |
100.0 |
144 |
100.0 |
148 |
100.0 |
437 |
100.0 |
a) Balanced
Participants verbalized that their age corresponded to their expectations at the present moment.
“I am aware that age is a game of gains and losses. We just have to find a real balance between both and make it a win-win process.” (Participant 43)
b) Old
Participants indicated that they were uneasy about their age and exemplified concerns about their age and their future.
“I feel old. I’m already 86 years, you know.” (Participant 144)
c) Youthful
A cognisant-young posture towards age was indicated by the participants.
“I do not feel old. I feel the same as I ever was.” (Participant 39)
“Being young is an attitude. If I believe that I am young, indeed I am young.” (Participant 48)
d) Dissatisfied
Participants verbalized discontent about their present age and that they felt old.
“I’m not happy about my age.” (Participant 44)
“I don’t like my age. I’m too old. I wish I was younger.” (Participant 67)
Cross-National Differences Analysis Concerning the Emergent Categories
Because our sample was cross-national, an Asymptotic Chi-Square Test was used to analyze if the three nationality groups diverged significantly concerning indicators of AtA and SA. As regards to the SA and its four categories, the results indicated significant differences between the three nationality groups (χ2(12) = 22.6, p < .05). Conversely, no significant differences between the three nationality groups were found regarding the total score for indicators of AtA. Yet, significant differences were found in four of the AtA categories: ‘sense of purpose and ambitions’ (χ2(8) = 24.5, p < .01); ‘health and wellness’ (χ2(10) = 18.0, p < .05); ‘existential meaning’ (χ2(6) = 27.4, p < .01) and ‘stability and accessibility’ (χ2(4) = 17.6, p < .01) (see Table 4).
Concerning the indicators of AtA, Portuguese scored higher in the ‘health and wellness’, ‘existential meaning’ and ‘stability and accessibility’ categories (37.4%, 43.9% and 43.0%, respectively) whilst Angolans indicated the highest results in ‘sense of purpose and ambitions’ (43.3%) and ‘social support’ (36.7%). In opposition, Romanians had the lowest score in ‘sense of purpose and ambitions’ (17.0%) and ‘health and wellness’ (29.6%). Portuguese scored the lowest in ‘social support’ (31.2%), while Angolans had the lowest scores in ‘existential meaning’ (23.7%) and ‘stability and accessibility’ (20.0%).
As regards to SA, Portuguese participants prompted the highest result in the ‘balanced’ category (42.5%) whereas Romanians participants scored the highest in the ‘old’ and ‘dissatisfied’ categories (52.1% and 50.0%, respectively). Angolans indicated the highest result for the ‘youthful’ category. Conversely Romanian displayed the lowest score in ‘balanced’ (21.3%), whilst Angolan indicated the lowest score in ‘old’ (10.6%) and ‘dissatisfied’ (24.1%). Finally, Portuguese scored the lowest in the ‘youthful’ category (21.7%).
Table 4
Chi-Square Test for the Cross-National Differences Among the Three Groups of Participants
Pearson Chi-Square Value |
df |
pa |
|
Indicators of AtA |
19.959 |
16 |
.206 |
Sense of Purpose and Ambitions |
24.514 |
8 |
.001 |
Health and Wellness |
18.010 |
10 |
.036 |
Social Support |
5.510 |
4 |
.240 |
Existential Meaning |
27.398 |
6 |
.000 |
Stability and Accessibility |
17.599 |
4 |
.001 |
Subjective Age |
22.581 |
12 |
.024 |
Balanced |
34.812 |
4 |
.000 |
Old |
51.298 |
4 |
.000 |
Youthful |
41.422 |
6 |
.000 |
Dissatisfied |
10.639 |
4 |
.022 |
a Monte Carlo Sig. (2-sided)
Discussion
This research was focused around two main research objectives: To analyze older adults’ perceptions of AtA and SA, and to assess differences among Portuguese, Romanian and Angolan participants concerning the cited concepts.
‘Existential meaning’ (26.3%) and ‘sense of purpose and ambitions’ (24.2%) were the most frequent indicators of AtA pointed out by this study’s participants. The prompting of objective (e.g. health) and subjective categories (e.g. existential meaning) that emerged from participants’ interviews, corroborated the multi-dimensional concept of AtA concept (Bauer & McAdams, 2004; Keyes, Shmotkin, & Ryff, 2002; Neri, Cachioni, & Resende, 2002).
As to SA, overall older adults expressed positive SA in 66.1% of overall narratives. ‘Balanced’ (36.5%) and ‘youthful’ (29.6%) were the most referred SA for older adults. As suggested by previous research, success in fulfilling challenges may yield more positive perceived age (Coupland, Coupland, Giles, & Henwood, 1991; Kleinspehn-Ammerlahn, Kotter-Grühn, & Smith, 2008). Moreover, growing literature indicates that “feeling younger” was rated more positively by older adults than younger adults (Giles, McIlrath, Mulac, & McCann, 2010). Moreover, older adults reported that they felt balanced and more agreeable than middle-aged and younger adults (Allemand, Zimprich, & Hendriks, 2008).
Considering the cross-national and cultural diversity of older adults’ perspectives concerning age (Barak, 2009), the highest scores in ‘health and wellness’, ‘existential meaning’, ‘stability and accessibility’ and ‘balanced’ were prompted by Portuguese participants whilst Angolans were accountable for the highest results in ‘sense of purpose and ambitions’, ‘social support’ and ‘youthful’. One possible explanation for these results could be the fact that Portuguese benefit from a stabilized social security system (Instituto Nacional Estatística, 2005) which might justify the highest scores for these categories and the focus on existential meaning, given that health, functional and environmental needs are currently met. Moreover, literature highpoints the development of a specific spirituality as people grow older (MacKinlay, 2001). Alternatively, Angolans mostly felt young-at-heart, and relied on social networking for psychological and social support. In fact, even when family support was not available, more varied forms of support were tapped to meet the needs of these participants (e.g., friends, professional peers, neighbors). Furthermore, we posited that a specific African definition of being old, in partly due to a recent past of war, poverty and subsequent existing young population (World Health Organization, 2003) might have led Angolan participants to higher valorize their sense of purpose and ambitions, as well as reinforcing their sense of social community. Furthermore, Romanians were accountable for the highest scores in ‘old’ and ‘dissatisfied’, which is in line with past literature that stresses the fact that Romanian elderly often enfold low prestige and social segregation (Popa, 2000).
‘Existential meaning’ was the most prevalent response for these participants. Significant differences were found for this category in the three nationalities. In particular, Portuguese and Romanian older adults emphasized this indicator of AtA. Instead, for Angolan participants, ‘sense of purpose and ambitions’ was the most reported indicator of AtA. Kotter-Grühn, Grühn, and Smith (2010) suggested that particularly very old adults seem to have quite accurate perceptions of their nearness to death. In addition, elderly people emphasize spiritual dimensions (Meuller, Plevak, & Rummans, 2001). In fact, for these older adults, existentialist and spiritual themes such as values and spiritual activities appeared to influence how participants led their lives, which corroborates what Tornstam (2005) named gerotranscendence, i.e., a forward and outward existential development that occurs in old age.
The sense of purpose and ambitions were considered very relevant for these older adults and significantly different for the three different groups. Thus, accomplishment, engagement and activities, particularly when in the company of professional peers, were pertinent for these participants. Previous studies indicated that older adults’ happiness variance can be partly attributed to the time seniors spend on various activities (Oerlemans, Bakker, & Veenhoven, 2011). Some cognitive functions—e.g., those involved in social functioning (Washburn, Sands, & Walton, 2003)—appear to augment with age and can compensate for those that may evidence decline. Intellectual and physical engagement have been found to increase the cognitive function of older adults and to provide an opportunity for social interactions and civic life, as well as to develop a sense of personal meaning (Park, Gutchess, Meade, & Stine-Morrow, 2007; Wilson et al., 2002). Conversely, in old age, with the greater likelihood of declining income and deteriorating health, positive SA and AtA can become difficult to achieve (Davidhizar & Shearer, 1999). Additionally, literature suggests that professional engagement especially with peers and productive creativity, contributes to positive SA and AtA (Brodsky, 1988; Stevens-Ratchford, 2005).
In spite of the high level of education of our participants (60.3% of the participants completed high school or above), health and wellness was not as highly reported as we initially expected. Moreover, we found significant differences for the three nationalities. Prior research points out that education is associated with improved knowledge about healthy behaviors and provides individuals with cognitive and psychosocial skills and material resources that allow them to effectively pursue health (Pampel, Krueger, & Denney, 2010). Furthermore, Hurd (2000) suggests the shift that occurs in priorities as people reach old age and that the emphasis on attractive appearance in the eyes of other people is replaced by one on health.
‘Social support’ was not highly indicated as contributing to AtA, as initially expected. Moreover and in opposition to the remaining categories, significant differences were not found for this category among the three nationalities. Considering that older adults live within a relatively steady social network which provides regular contact over time, difficulties in social networks is partially attributed to functional loss, health disparities and the discontinuation of personal relationships (Lang, 2001). Additionally, Bowling (2007) and Birren and Schaie (1996) reported health and interpersonal relationships to be relevant for older adults.
Interestingly, ‘stability and accessibility’ was not a predominant response in our study. Conversely, the importance of stability has been previously identified in literature (Birren & Schaie, 1996; Low & Molzahn, 2007). It is likely that the high level of activity of our participants (43%), in combination with the existence of a social pension scheme for older adults, have influenced the participants’ perceptions. Diverse income portfolios suggest long-term financial security and are associated with longer lives because individuals can prosper even if any single income source should fail (Krueger & Burgard, 2011). Moreover, material well-being peaks in individuals’ final years in the labor force and then provides a long-term and relatively stable resource in the remaining decades of life (Haas, Krueger, & Rohlfsen, 2012).
Notably, there are significant differences in the SA and in its all categories. As to the indicators of AtA, we found significant differences in all indicators of AtA categories, except for ‘social support’. Taken together, our results lend further support to the cross-national and cultural diversity of older adults’ experiences concerning age, which corroborates prior literature emphasizing the fact that cultural and ethnical differences may influence the process of aging (Barak, 2009; Torres, 2003).
Our study offers a valuable contribution to the aging literature. However, the results need to be interpreted within the context of the following limitations. Despite the fact that a varied sample of participants was recruited, the use of a purposeful method could have resulted in some selection bias. Yet, biased results were minimized by the checking of the findings with external review and with the respondents themselves. Moreover, this study has, enabled an insight into the typologies of categories that had impact on AtA and SA for elderly people. One-on-one interviews have the potential to provide in-depth information. If the researchers had begun by simply collecting rating scale data, they would have risked developing survey items that were grounded more in investigators’ preconceptions about elderly people than in the constructs they actually consider most meaningful (Reichstadt, 2010).
Further research is needed into the conceptual framework of AtA for older adults. The current study extends previous research by giving empirical weight to what actually constitutes one’s personal perception of AtA and SA and to the conceptual links between age and aging. Moreover, our findings support the view that there are cross-national differences regarding SA and AtA. Regardless of the constraints of personal competence and resources among older adults when facing old age, we assert that the findings of this study are a pertinent input for understanding the importance of the cross-national context of age and aging and for optimizing personal functioning and well-being in older adults from different cultural backgrounds.
References
Allemand, M., Zimprich, D., & Hendriks, A. A. J. (2008). Age differences in five personality domains across the life span. Developmental Psychology, 44(3), 758-770. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.44.3.758
Al-Janabi, H. N., Flynn, T., & Coast, J. (2012). Development of a self-report measure of capability wellbeing for adults: The ICECAP-A. Quality of Life Research, 21(1), 167-176. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11136-011-9927-2
Baltes, P. B., & Baltes, M. M. (1993). Psychological perspectives on successful ageing: the model of selective optimization with compensation. In: Baltes PB, Baltes MM, (Eds.), Successful Aging: Perspectives From the Behavioral Sciences. New York: Cambridge University Press, 1-27. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511665684.003
Baltes, P. B., & Smith, J. (2003). New frontiers in the future of aging: From successful aging of the young old to the dilemmas of the fourth age. Gerontology, 49, 123–135. http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000067946
Barak, B. (2009). Age identity: A cross-cultural global approach. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 33, 2–11. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0165025408099485
Bauer, J. J., & McAdams, D. P. (2004). Growth goals, maturity, and well-being. Developmental Psychology, 40(1), 114–127. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.40.1.114
Birren, J. E., & Schaie, K. W. (1996). Handbook of the psychology of aging. San Diego: Academic Press.
Bowling, A. (2007). Aspirations for older age in the 21st century: What is successful aging? International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 64, 263-297. http://dx.doi.org/10.2190/L0K1-87W4-9R01-7127
Brandtstädter, J., & Renner, G. (1990). Tenacious goal pursuit and flexible goal adjustment: Explication and age-related analysis of assimilation and accommodation strategies of coping. Psychology and Aging, 5, 58-67. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0882-7974.5.1.58
Brandtstädter, J., Wentura, D., & Greve, W. (1993). Adaptive resources of the aging self: Outlines of an emergent perspective. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 16, 323-349.
Brodsky, S. L. (1988). The psychology of adjustment and well-being. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.
Brozek, J. (1966). Symposium on adjustment to aging. Applied Psychology, 15, 22–29. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1464-0597.1966.tb00581.x
Coupland, J., Coupland, N., Giles, H., & Henwood, K. (1991). Formulating age: Dimensions of age identity in elderly talk. Discourse Processes, 14(1), 87–106. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01638539109544776
Corner, L., Brittain, K., & Bond, J. (2007). Social aspects of ageing. Psychiatry, 6(12), 480-483. http://dx.doi.org/10.1383/psyt.3.12.5.56782
Fernández-Ballesteros, R. (2007). GeroPsychology. European Perspectives for and Ageing World. Gottingen: Hogrefe & Huber.
Fernández-Ballesteros, R. (2010). Quality of life in old age: Problematic issues. Applied Research Quality Life, 6, 21–40.http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11482-010-9110-x
Filion, P., Wister, A., & Coblentz, E. J. (1993). Subjective dimensions of environmental adaptation among the elderly. Journal of Housing for the Elderly, 10, 3-32. http://dx.doi.org/10.1300/J081v10n01_02
Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975). Mini-mental state. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189–198.
Ford, A. B., Haug, M. R., Stange, K. C., Gaines, A. D., Noelker, L. S., & Jones, P. K. (2000). Sustained personal autonomy: a measure of successful aging. Journal Aging Health,12, 470-489. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/089826430001200402
Freund, A. M., & Baltes, P. B. (1998). Selection, optimization, and compensation as strategies of life management: correlations with subjective indicators of successful aging. Psychology and Aging, 13(4), 531-543. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.13.4.531
Gavrilov, L. A., & Heuveline, P. (2003). Aging of Population. In P. Demeny and G. McNicoll (Eds.) The Encyclopedia of Population. New York: Macmillan Reference USA. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4020-8356-3_6
Giles, H., McIlrath, M., Mulac, A., & McCann, R. M. (2012). Expressing age salience: three generations’ reported events, frequencies, and valences. International Journal of the Sociology of Language, 206, 73-96 http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/ijsl.2010.049
Haas, S. A., Krueger, P. M., & Rohlfsen, L. (2012). Race/ethnic and nativity disparities in later life physical performance: the role of health and socioeconomic status over the life course. Journal of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 67B(2), 238-48. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbr155
Headey, B., & Wearing, A. (1989). Personality, life events, and subjective well-being: Toward a dynamic equilibrium model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 731–739. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.57.4.731
Heidrich, S. M., & Ryff, C. D. (1993). The role of social comparisons processes in the psychological adaptation of elderly adults. Journal of Gerontology, 48, 127–136.
Hurd, L. C. (2000). Older women’s body image and embodied experience: An exploration. Journal of Women and Aging, 12, 77-97. http://dx.doi.org/10.1300/J074v12n03_06
Instituto Nacional de Estatística. (2000). As gerações mais idosas [The older generations]. Lisboa: DECP /Serviço de estudos sobre a população.
Jopp, D. & Rott. C. (2006). Adaptation in very old age: exploring the role of resources, beliefs, and attitudes for centenarians’ happiness. Psychology and Aging, 21(2), 266–280. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.21.2.266
Kesebir, P., & Diener, E. (2010). In pursuit of happiness: Empirical answers to philosophical questions. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3, 117–125. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1758-0854.2012.01066.x
Keyes, C. L. M., Shmotkin, D., & Ryff, C. D. (2002). Optimizing well-being: The empirical encounter of two traditions.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(6), 1007-1022. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.82.6.1007
Kleinspehn-Ammerlahn, A., Kotter-Grühn, D., & Smith, J. (2008). Self-perceptions of aging: Do subjective age and satisfaction with aging change during old age? Journal of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 63B(6), 377–385.http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/63.6.P377
Knight, T., & Ricciardelli, L. A. (2003). Successful aging: perceptions of adults aged between 70 and 101 years. International Journal Aging and Human Development, 56, 223-245. http://dx.doi.org/10.2190/CG1A-4Y73-WEW8-44QY
Kotter-Grühn, D., Grühn, D., & Smith, J. (2010).Predicting one’s own death: the relationship between subjective and objective nearness to death in very old age. European Journal of Ageing, 7(4), 293-300.http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10433-010-0165-1
Krueger, P. M., & Burgard, S. A. (2011). Work, occupation, income, and mortality. In. R. G. Rogers & E. M. Crimmins (Eds.), International Handbook of Adult Mortality (pp. 263-288). New York, NY: Springer. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-90-481-9996-9_13
Kuh, D. (2007). New Dynamics of Ageing Preparatory Network. A life course approach to healthy aging, frailty, and capability. Journal Gerontology Series A: Biological Sciences Medical Sciences, 62(7), 717-721.
Lang, F. (2001). Regulation of social relationships in later adulthood. Journal of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 56B(6), 321-326. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/56.6.P321
Low, G., & Molzahn, A. E. (2007). Predictors of quality of life in old age: A cross-validation study. Research in Nursing & Health, 30(2), 141-50.http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/nur.20178
MacKinlay, E. (2001). The spiritual dimension of caring: applying a model for spiritual tasks of ageing. Journal of Religious Gerontology, 12(3/4), 151-166. http://dx.doi.org/10.1300/J078v12n03_12
Meuller, P. S., Plevak, D. J., & Rummans, T. A. (2001). Religious involvement, spirituality and medicine: Implications for clinical practice. Mayo Clinic Proceedings, 76, 1225–1235. http://dx.doi.org/10.4065/76.12.1225
Montepare, J. M. (2009). Subjective age: Toward a guiding lifespan framework. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 33(1), 42–46.http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0165025408095551
Neri, A. L., Cachioni, M. & Resende, M. C. (2002). Atitudes em relação à velhice [Attitudes towards old age]. In Freitas, E. V., Py, L., Neri, A. L., Cançado, F. A. X., Gorzoni, M. L., Rocha, S. M. Tratado de Geriatria e Gerontologia. Rio de Janeiro: Guanabara-Koogan, p. 972-980.
Oerlemans, W. G. M., Bakker, A. B, & Veenhoven, R. (2011). Finding the key to happy aging: A day reconstruction study of happiness. Journal of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 66B(6), 665-674.http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbr040
Pampel, F. C., Krueger, P. M., & Denney, J. T. (2010). Socioeconomic disparities in health behaviors. Annual Review of Sociology, 36, 349-370. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.soc.012809.102529
Palmore, E. B. (2001). The ageism survey: First findings. Gerontologist, 41(5), 572-575. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geront/41.5.572
Park, D. C., Gutchess, A. H., Meade, M. L., & Stine-Morrow, E. A. L. (2007). Improving cognitive function in older adults: Nontraditional approaches. Journals of Gerontology: Series B (62B), 45–52. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/62.special_issue_1.45
Pavot, W., & Diener, E. (2004). The subjective evaluation of well-being in adulthood: Findings and implications. Ageing International, 29(2), 113–135.http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12126-004-1013-4
Popa, A. (2000). The social perception of the elder. Between objectivity and distortion - Exploratory Study. Quality of life, 12, 91-100.
Reichstadt, J., Sengupta, G., Depp C. A., Palinkas, L. A., & Jeste, D. V. (2010). Older adults’ perspectives on successful aging: Qualitative interviews. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 18(7), 567-75.
Ryff, C. D., & Keyes, C. L. M. (1995). The structure of psychological well-being revisited. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69,719–727. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.69.4.719
Shmotkin, D. (1998). Declarative and differential aspects of subjective well-being and implications for mental health in later life. In J. Lomranz (Ed.), Handbook of aging and mental health: An integrative approach (pp. 15–43). New York: Plenum.
Slangen-Dekort, Y. A. W., Midden, J. B. C., Aarts, B., & Wagenberg, F. V. (2001). Determinants of adaptive behavior among older persons: Self-efficacy, importance, and personal disposition as directive mechanisms. International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 53(4), 253-274.
Sneed, J. R., & Whitbourne, S. K. (2003). Identity processing and self-consciousness in middle and later adulthood. Journals of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 58(6), 313-319. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/58.6.P313
Staudinger, U. M., & Kunzmann, U. (2005). Positive adult personality development: adjustment and/or growth? European Psychologist, 10(4), 320–329. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1016-9040.10. 4.320
Stephan, Y., Caudroit, J., & Chalabaev. A. (2011). Subjective health and memory self-efficacy as mediators in the relation between subjective age and life satisfaction among older adults. Aging & Mental Health, 15(4), 428–436. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13607863.2010.536138
Stevens-Ratchford, R. G. (2005). Occupational engagement: Motivation for older adult participation. Topics in Geriatric Rehabilitation, 21(23), 171-181.
Thomae, T. (1992). Contributions of longitudinal research to a cognitive theory of adjustment to aging. European Journal of Personality, 6(2), 157–175. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/per.2410060207
Tornstam, L. (2005). Gerotranscendence: A developmental theory of positive aging. New York, NY: Springer Publishing Company.
Torres, S. (2003). A preliminary empirical test of a culturally-relevant theoretical framework for the study of successful aging. Journal of Cross-Cultural Gerontology, 18(1), 79-100.
United Nations, Department of Economic and Social Affairs, Population Division. (1999). The world population prospects: the 1998 revision. New York: Author.
United Nations. (2009). World Population Prospects: The 2008 Revision. CD-ROM Edition - Extended Dataset, United Nations.
United Nations, Department of Economic and Social Affairs, Population Division. (2011). World Population Prospects: The 2010 Revision. New York: Author.
United States Bureau of the Census. (2000). International Data Base (IDB). International Program Center, Washington DC. http://www.census.gov/ipc/ www/idbnew.html
Von Humboldt, S., Leal, I., & Pimenta, F. (2012). Assessing subjective age and adjustment to aging in a Portuguese and German older population: A comparative multiple correspondence analysis. International Journal of Psychological Studies, 4(2), 141-153. http://dx.doi.org/10.5539/ijps.v4n2p141
Westerhof, G. J., & Barrett, A. E. (2005). Age identity and subjective well-being: A comparison of the United States and Germany. Journal of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 60B(3), 129-136. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/60.3.S129
Washburn, A. M., Sands, L. P., & Walton, P. (2003). Assessment of social cognition in frail seniors and its association with social functioning in the nursing home. The Gerontologist. 43, 203-212.
Wilson, R. S., de Leon, C. F. M., Barnes, L. L., Schneider, J. S., Bienias, J. L., Evans, D. A. & Bennett, D. A. (2002). Participation in cognitively stimulating activities and risk of incident Alzheimer Disease. JAMA, 287,742-748. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/jama.287.6.742
World Health Organization. (2003). Directory of Research on Ageing in Africa: 1995-2003. Geneve, Switzerland: Author.
Refbacks
- There are currently no refbacks.
Copyright (c)
Reminder
- How to do online submission to another Journal?
- If you have already registered in Journal A, then how can you submit another article to Journal B? It takes two steps to make it happen:
Submission Guidelines for Canadian Social Science
We are currently accepting submissions via email only. The registration and online submission functions have been disabled.
Please send your manuscripts to [email protected],or [email protected] for consideration. We look forward to receiving your work.
Articles published in Canadian Social Science are licensed under Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 (CC-BY).
Canadian Social Science Editorial Office
Address: 1020 Bouvier Street, Suite 400, Quebec City, Quebec, G2K 0K9, Canada.
Telephone: 1-514-558 6138
Website: Http://www.cscanada.net; Http://www.cscanada.org
E-mail:[email protected]; [email protected]
Copyright © Canadian Academy of Oriental and Occidental Culture